From: Christopher B Reeve <[cr 39] at [andrew.cmu.edu]> Organization: Sophomore, Electrical and Computer Engineering, Carnegie Mellon, Pittsburgh, PA "In February 1990, for example, the National Transportation Safety Board released a study that board members described as the most detailed ever conducted of drug and alcohol abuse in interstate trucking. The New York Times ran its report of the study under this headline: 'Truck Deaths Linked to Alcohol or Drugs.' Reporting the findings that Safety Board members called significant, the Times reporter quoted a board member as saying, 'I expected to find higher alcohol and drug use than the industry and the regulatory agencies have predicted. But I did not expect to find 33 percent' [Cushman, John H., Jr. (2/7/90). Truck Deaths Linked to Alcohol or Drugs. New York Times: A11.]. However, if the study is examined, one finds that the results are far from significant; they are utterly useless. The data were collected from autopsies performed on truck drivers who had been killed in accidents in eight states. One-third of them had recently used what was described as 'drugs and alcohol' [Cushman, John H., Jr. (2/7/90). Truck Deaths Linked to Alcohol or Drugs. New York Times: A11.]. But there was no control group. Theoretically, 99 percent of truckers who did not have accidents could have recently used drugs or alcohol. Even the Transportation Department itself has admitted that no reliable studies linked drug use to serious accidents in the trucking industry. After a review of studies designed to determine the relationship between drug use and highway accidents, the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration admitted that 'the nature and extent to which drugs, other than alcohol, are a serious highway safety problem cannot be specified with certainty' [Salpukas, Agis. (1/27/90). Roadblock for Random Drug Tests. New York Times: 21.]." (Christina Jacqueline Johns, Power, Idealogy, and the War on Drugs: Nothing Succeeds Like Failure, 97) "There are many reasons why widespread private sector drug testing is unlikely to have a signficant impact on drug use or accidents thought to be related to drug use. First, some drug userse are unemployed. Second, most companies give advance warning of drug testing, which allows those using drugs to either stop use for a while or switch to less detectable drugs. There are even substances on the market that mask the traces of some drugs (e.g., steroids) for the few hours of the test [National Public Radio. (3/5/91). Morning Edition.]. As Zimmer [Zimmer, Lynn. (1989). Employment Drug Testing and Its Effectiveness in the War against Drugs. Paper Presented at the annual meeting of the American Society of Criminology, Reno, Nevada.] has noted, folk wisdom is already replete with stories of how to beat the drug tests. There is also some indication that drug testing (like interdiction efforts) often has the effect of merely pushing the problem around. When employers begin to test, workers move to other employers in the area who are not testing [Roberts, Preggy. (2/19/90). More Companies Say Yes to Drug Tests for Workers. Montgomery Advertiser: 1.]. The APT Foundation report noted that testing alone, even of employees in 'safety-sensitive' positions, was unlikely to provide adequate protection from accidents [Wicker, Tom. (12/1/89). Warning about Tests. New York Times: 31.]." (Christina Jacqueline Johns, Power, Idealogy, and the War on Drugs: Nothing Succeeds Like Failure, 98 - 99) "In late 1990, the FBI announced that it would begin field testing kits that would make it possible for police to test for the handling of drugs by wiping the palms of stopped drivers. This method was described as offering the advantage of being 'less intrusive' (and therefore presumably more palatable) than other testing methods. The test uses antibodies that detect small amounts of the target substance on skin, desk tops, or other surfaces. It is argued that the tests are so highly calibrated that they do not pick up the tiny amounts of cocaine that are now evidently on virtually all $20 and $50 bills in circulation [Montgomery Advertiser. (8/30/90). New Tests Detect Drugs Instantly: 4A.]. Similarly, New Hampshire has been using a chemical that police drop on the driver's licenses of people whom they have stopped. If the chemical indicates the presence of drugs, the police used it as a legal justification for searching the driver and the car. However, the president of the New Hampshire Criminal Defense Lawyers Association, which filed charges against the state for the practice, maintains that licenses may test positive when there is no cocaine present. Laboratory tests conducted by the Lawyers Association showed that thousands of other chemicals, including those in laundry detergents, produced the same reaction [Lewin, Tamar. (9/13/90). Drug-Testing Kit for Parents Spurs Stormy Debate. New York Times: 12A.]." (Christina Jacqueline Johns, Power, Idealogy, and the War on Drugs: Nothing Succeeds Like Failure, 99) "In a Washington Post / ABC News poll conducted in 1989, 55 percent of the respondents advocated mandatory drug tests for all U.S. citizens. Sixty-seven percent agreed that all high school students should be regularly tested for drugs [Morin, Richard. (9/8/89). Many in Poll Say Bush Plan Is Not Stringent Enough. Washington Post: A18.]. The ACLU has persuaded some states to pass laws prohibiting drug testing unless there is reason to suspect a particular individual. But at least one bill has been introduced in Congress that would override such state laws and allow employer drug testing of virtually any employee [American Civil Liberties Union. (1989). Letter. New York.]." (Christina Jacqueline Johns, Power, Idealogy, and the War on Drugs: Nothing Succeeds Like Failure, 100) The Stepping Stone / Substitution / Gateway Theories [In response to gateway theories: In fact, probably the most common reason for moving on to more harmful or potentially dangerous substances of abuse is a rise in price or drop in availability of the currently used substance.] "The absurdity of most of the positions on the 'stepping stone' hypothesis can be seen by examining some of the ways authors cited each other as their sole supporting evidence for the theory. One F. R. Gomila, who was a colleague and co-worker with Fossier, wrote a book about marihuana, which he condensed into an article in 1938. R. P. Walton reprinted Gomila's article (on the 'stepping-stone' hypothesis) in his book, Marihuana, America's New Drug Problem. In 1943 P. O. Wolff used Walton's reprint as the sole supporting evidence in an article advocating the 'stepping-stone' hypothesis. Another article that advanced the 'stepping-stone' hypothesis appeared in 1937, listing Fossier's article as one of the five bibliographic references given. In all, there were only 15 works printed during the period 1931 to 1948 that advanced this hypothesis." (J. Mandel, "Who Says Marijuana Use Leads to Heroin Addiction?" Journal of Secondary Education, 43 (1968), 212, extracted from Lester Grinspoon, Marihuana Reconsidered, 237) "California has also kept statistics for drug law violators who have not had a past record of involvvement with opiates, and from an examination of these data, Mandel has reached the following conclusions: Dangerous drugs (amphetamines, barbituates, etc.) are more often 'starters' towards opiate than is marijuana. ... Opiate use without a past history of marijuana is seven times likelier than opiat use with a past record of pot smoking. ... On the average, over 60 times as many Californians are arrested for marijuana without having a history of 'hard drugs' than appear to 'graduate' from marijuana to heroin. As Mandel notes, each of these could be used to refute the 'stepping-stone' view." (J. Mandel, "Who Says Marijuana Use Leads to Heroin Addiction?" Journal of Secondary Education, 43 (1968), 215, extracted from Lester Grinspoon, Marihuana Reconsidered, 243) "in 1950 the Federal Bureau of Narcotics ran a random study of 602 cases pulled from their files of opiate convictions and found that only 'seven per cent off them started on marihuana.' Again, the indication is that the assumption that previous marihuana use is a causative factor is entirely spurious. The Task Force on Narcotics and Drug Abuse of the President's Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice asserted in 1967 that approximately 50 percent of heroin users have had some prior experience with marihuana. However, this same group noted that most of the heroin users studied had also had previous experience with alcohol and tobacco. It might have been noted that most of them had had experience with milk, Coca-Cola, or sex. Such retrospective studies of this problem give no clue as to the existence of any conceivable typical 'escalation'; there is simply no valid evidence of anythinginherent in cannabis or cannabis use which would make the marihuana user likely to become a heroin or other opiate user. Marihuana is, as far as has yet been determined a precursor of only further marihuana use." (two sources: U.S. Congress, House, Ways and Means Committe, Control of Narcotics, Marihuana, and Barbiturates, Hearings on H.R. 3490 and H.R. 348, 82nd Cong., 1st sess., April 7, 14, and 17, 1951, statement of George W. Cunningham, Deputy Commissioner, Bureau of Narcotics, Treasury Department, p. 64; and second source: Task Force on Narcotics and Drug Abuse, The President's Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice, Task Force Report, Narcotics and Drug Abuse (Annotation and Consultants' Papers) (Washington, 1967), p. 13. All of this is extracted from Lester Grinspoon, Marihuana Reconsidered, 244) "There is no evidence of a causal connection between the two drugs [marijuana and heroin], and all attempts to establish the truth of the progression theory have failed. The theory was explicitly denied in the 1937 marijuana hearings, but drug progression again became a matter of public concern in the 1940s and 1950s when an alleged increase in heroin addiction was attributed to prior marijuana use. The main support fot the theory comes from studies showing a shistory of marijuana use in addicts. As Pet and Ball show [Pet, Donald D., and John C. Ball. "Marijuana Smoking in the U.S.," Federal Probation, Sept. 1968], 80.4 percent of addicts at the U.S. Public Health Service Hospital at Lexington, Kentucky, had smoked marijuana. But this post hoc reasoning does not stand up. By the time these people became heroin addicts they had tried everything, including liquor and milk. The real question is, what percentage of marijuana users become heroin addicts? The answer appears to be very few indeed. In his extensive sduty of student drug use in 1968 [Blum, Richard H. Students and Drugs, San Francisco, Jossey-Bass, 1969], Blum found that only 7 percent of maijuana users had tried opiates; making adjustments for the subsequent upsurge in marijuana use, this figure would now be around 3 percent. Blum, examing the population of the Oakland ghetto, found that teen-age heroin and marijuana users belonged to completely diffeent grousp who held different values. Few of the 'heads' turned to heroin. These reports were confirmed by the marijuana users we interviewed, very few of whom were willing to try heroin." (Norman E. Zinberg and John A. Robertson, Drugs & The Public, 40) "In any case, the relevant quesiton is what proportion of marijuana users become addicts, not what proportionof addicts first used marijuana. We would probably find that 100 percent of addicts first used tobacco, alcohol, and even milk, but these are not responsible for heroin addiction. The best statistics on this subject came from California, where marijuana use has grown more rapidly than elsewhere. But these figures show no corresponding rise in heroin addiction. In fact, marijuana use is quite prevalent among both college students and North Africans, two groups with very low rates of opiate addiction. The evidentiary picture is summed up in a report to the British Home Office on the dangers of marijuana, which put the matter in these terms: 'It can clearly be argued on the world picture that cannabis use does not lead to heroin addiction. [Wootton Report, p. 12] " (Norman E. Zinberg and John A. Robertson, Drugs & The Public, 181) "Stepped-up enforcement also frequently leads to substitution. In July 1990, for example, participants in a conference held by NIDA were maintaining decreases in cocaine use and no evidence of substitution. By August 1990, however, after more than a decade of decline, heroin use in some cities was on the rise. Federal drug authorities were saying in August 1990 that heroin was then more abundant and cheaper and was sold in purer forms. In addition, according to DEA estimates, between 1985 and 1989 the world production of opium more than doubled [Belsie, Laurent. (8/1/90). Concern Grows over Heroin Use. Christian Science Monitor: 6.]." (Christina Jacqueline Johns, Power, Idealogy, and the War on Drugs: Nothing Succeeds Like Failure, 10) Drug-related Violence "In 1962 L. Kolb noted that the alleged crime-inducing properties of cannabis had been studied at length in at least five well-conducted investigations by competent scientists or doctors in this country, and none of them had found that there was any evidence for a link between cannabis and aggressive crime, and, in particular, none of the investigators had found any evidence for anything that could possibly be labeled a cannabis-induced murder. Kolb believes that the gneral harmfulness of the drug has been grossly overrated by the antimarihuana laws and their proponents: 'Marihuana, like alcohol, releases the user's inhibitions and distorts his judgment ..., but its potency as an instigator of crime has not been ... demonstrated in the United States ... The tendency to credit a narcotic as the cause of physical, mental, and social disasters is so great in the United States that marihuana-induced crimes are often reported in the press and by police-trained people when there is no causal relation of marihuana to the crime.' As an example, he gives a case in which two young men consumed a large amount of whiskey in a hotel room, then smoked one marihuana cigarette, began to quarrel, fought, and one killed the other. The press played up the story as an example of a vicious, marihuana-induced murder." (L. Kolb, Drug Addiction: A Medical Problem (Springfield, Ill., 1962), pp. 23-24, extracted from Lester Grinspoon, Marihuana Reconsidered, 310) "As we have said, it is also widely believed that the drug user becomes morally enslaved and falls very easily into criminal activity. In this view, the drug turns its user into something akin to the ordinary criminal. Here again we will distinguish between the dependency-prone and the drug-experimenting groups. Studies [Gerard, D. L., and C. Kornestsky. "Adolescent Opiate Addiction: A Study of Control and Addict Subjects," Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 29, 1955; Hill, H.E., C.A. Haertzen, and R. Glaser. "Personality Characteristics of Narcotic Addicts as Indicated by the MMPI," Journal of General Psychology, Vol. 62, 1960; Alksne, H., et al., "A Follow-Up Study of Treated Adolescent Narcotic Users," New York: unpublished report of Columbia University School of Public Health and Admin. Med., 1959, quoted in Vaillant, G., "A Twelve-year Follow-Up of New York Narcotic Addicts. III. Some Social and Psychiatric Characteristics." Archives of Genreal Psychology, Vol. 15, 1966; Crime and Delinquency in California, Bureau of Criminal Statistics, State of California, 1967] of the first group [people who resort to drug use because of personality disorders] demonstatre that the hard-core user was criminal before he began to use drugs." (Norman E. Zinberg and John A. Robertson, Drugs & The Public, 45) "a careful study of the studies done linking marijuana to aggression or crime concludes that there is strong evidence against the link between marijuana and crime. The survey analyzes several famous cases, which have been repeatedly quoted to illustrate the criminogenic effect of the drug. Tenuousness of the evidence is revealed in each instance. Other large studies (Indian Hemp Commission, La Guardia Report, Blum, Blumer, police/arrest figures) consistently find that, if anything, marijuana inhibits such antisocial activity [Kaplan, John. Marijuana, The New Prohibition, New York, World Publishing Co., 1969]. The marijuana user is criminal only in that he uses an illegal drug. The modern marijuana user usually comes from a social class different from that of the heroin addict, and he uses the drug differently; it is as much part of his ordinary social life as a martini in the evening is for his father." (Norman E. Zinberg and John A. Robertson, Drugs & The Public, 46) "Senator Thomas Dodd, chariman of the Senate Subcommittee on Juvenile Delinquency - the committee where much drug legislation originates - told the Senate of information he had heard [Boston Herald Traveler, Dec. 5, 1969] ... from an outstanding expert that the marijuana toxic psychosis ... may have played a part in the events at My Lai on March 16, 1968 ... I plan to conduct hearings to get at the facts, to let our people know if our soldiers in Vietnam have suddenly become brutal stormtroopers or whether, as I consider more likely, some of them have become victims of a drug problem that has already torn asunder the fabric of domestic American society. That Senator Dodd can make this unproven and unwarranted statement without fear of contradiction from responsible quarters is a glaring example of smokescreening. The harsh realiteis of the Vietnam War are hidden in the miasma of drug use." (Norman E. Zinberg and John A. Robertson, Drugs & The Public, 238) -- "Freud was convinced that 'the voice of the intellect will be heard.' But no one understood better than he that if reason is to triumph, it has to sound above the clamor of conflicting emotion and the roar of primitive desires." (Zinberg and Robertson, _Drugs & The Public_, 242)